Journal of Fungal Diversity

Journal of Fungal Diversity

Journal of Fungal Diversity

Current Issue Volume No: 1 Issue No: 1

Research Article Open Access Available online freely Peer Reviewed Citation

Variation of Ethnomycological Knowledge in a Community from Central Mexico

1Posgrado en Ciencias Biológicas, Instituto de Geografía, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. Circuito exterior, Ciudad Universitaria, Apartado Postal 04510, Ciudad de México.

2Instituto de Geología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. Circuito de la Investigación Científica s/n, Ciudad Universitaria, Coyoacán, C.P. 02376, Ciudad de México.

3Laboratorio de Biodiversidad, Centro de Investigaciones en Ciencias Biológicas, Universidad Autónoma de Tlaxcala

4Instituto de Geografía, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Circuito exterior, Ciudad Universitaria, Apartado Postal 04510, Ciudad de México, Ciudad de México.

5Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Avenida Universidad 3000, Circuito Exterior s/n, Ciudad Universitaria, Coyoacán, C.P. 04510, Ciudad de México.

Abstract

We analyze the effects of the ethnomycological knowledge depending on the age range of its users and how far their homes are from the forest areas. As a study model, the traditional mycological knowledge of San Pedro Tlalcuapan SPT, in Central Mexico was used. During 2017, 135 semi-structured interviews were conducted with people of three age ranges and living in three areas of the community located at different distances from the forest. Data was analyzed using descriptive statistics and analyses of non-metric multidimensional scaling, which were then applied to calculate two indices. The effect of age and origin of the individuals on bio-cultural relevance was assessed using multiple linear regression models.Fifty species of edible wild mushrooms were registered, those mentioned most often were of the Russuladelica complex., Amanita basii and Boletus aff. edulis. Ninety-five percent of the people interviewed consumed wild mushrooms on average two times a week, while 57% collected them. Wild mushrooms are mainly recognized for their food and ecological importance. Ordination and regression analyses show that people closest to the forest, as well as older people, are able to identify more of the wild mushrooms and have greater bio-cultural mushroom awareness. Results show that there is a risk that traditional knowledge about wild mushrooms will disappear, since it is only popular in a part of the community and only at some age ranges, indicating that there is a disconnect in the transfer of knowledge.

Author Contributions
Received 19 Mar 2019; Accepted 28 May 2019; Published 05 Jun 2019;

Academic Editor: Samantha Chandranath Karunarathna, Kunming Institute of Botany, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Kunming, China

Checked for plagiarism: Yes

Review by: Single-blind

Copyright ©  2019 Eribel Bello-Cervantes, et al.

License
Creative Commons License     This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Competing interests

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Citation:

Eribel Bello-Cervantes, Alexander Correa-Metrio, Adriana Montoya, Irma Trejo, Joaquín Cifuentes Blanco (2019) Variation of Ethnomycological Knowledge in a Community from Central Mexico. Journal of Fungal Diversity - 1(1):6-26. https://doi.org/10.14302/issn.2766-869X.jfd-19-2718

Download as RIS, BibTeX, Text (Include abstract )

DOI 10.14302/issn.2766-869X.jfd-19-2718

Introduction

Bio-cultural heritage is maintained by traditional knowledge, which is a belief system (cosmos), knowledge (corpus) and practice (praxis) that have been generated, specialized, shared and transferred across generations in native communities, according to the characteristics and the needs of each people. This knowledge is transferred through conversation. This transference is weakened and may possibly become extinct if communities no longer reassess it, use it and pass it on 1, 2.

Different socieconomic pressures have resulted in a series of modifications in the use of natural resources, which has threatened and endangered bio-cultural diversity 3, 4, 5. It has been observed a cultural change driven by the formal education systems 6, 7, 8, the integration of local communities in the market economies 9; the restriction on access to resources through conservation programs 10, in addition to the processes of modernization that include, introduction of technology, urbanization and modern health services 11.

This cultural change has been reflected in the variation of the distribution of traditional knowledge. The age, gender and profession are some factors that influence the diversity and richness of this knowledge (12. It has been documented that older persons embrace greater local knowledge 13. As well, persons involved in few or no environment-related activities have less traditional knowledge 12, 14, 15). In other words, elderly people and/or those connected with the environment are currently protecting this traditional environmental knowledge and can pass on to other generations.

Wild mushrooms play an important ecological 16 and cultural role in many communities, from pre-Hispanic times. Even, today, they continue to be a valuable forest resource that provide much-needed income for many families in the world 17. Various studies have described differences in the ethnomycology knowledge within communities or by gender 18, 19, 20, 21), and by age 22, 23. So far, however, we did not have the tools to discover the causes and ways in which traditional mycological knowledge about mushrooms is distributed.

San Pedro Tlalcuapan (SPT) is a community of indigenous nahua ancestry, located in the slopes of the National Park La Malinche (PNLM) in Tlaxcala, Mexico. Activities, such as collecting mushroom and edible or medicinal plants contributed to the generation of knowledge, ideas and myths, as well as to the local economy. Currently, natural resources are dwindling, resulting in a change and detachment from traditional ecological knowledge. Concerned about this situation, local authorities and members of the community have shown interest in reaffirming and supporting the conservation of their bio-cultural heritage.

This paper, therefore, explores the effect that the distance from a house to the forest has on the preservation of traditional knowledge as well as the change of knowledge according to age. Then can we develop a strategy for the conservation of the area’s mycological resources and the knowledge about them.

Materials and Methods

Study Area

The community of SPT, located in the Northwest part of the PNLM (19 ° 16' 50.02 'N, 98 ° 09' 0630' W), is a municipality of Santa Ana Chiautempan, in the State of Tlaxcala, Mexico (Figure 1). It has an altitude of 2,411 meters above sea level, in an area characterized by precipitation averaging 901 mm a year and annual temperatures averaging 15.75 ° C. The community is surrounded by pine forests, pine-oak forests, oak-pine forests, each with different levels of conservation, as well as man-introduced grassland and temporary agriculture 24.

Figure 1.Location of the SPT in the National Park La Malinche Tlaxcala, Mexico. Top left of the image represents the location of the National Park La Malinche inside Mexico. Top right, represents SPT within the national park. In the lower part of the image, the 4 main areas of the studio area are shown.
 Location of the SPT in the National Park La Malinche Tlaxcala, Mexico. Top left of the image               represents the location of the National Park La Malinche inside Mexico. Top right, represents SPT within the national park. In the lower part of the image, the 4 main areas of the studio area are shown.

San Pedro Tlalcuapan extends along 1,162 hectares, of which approximately 600 are covered by crops and forest; the rest belongs to the local population, which is divided into three main areas: La Defensa, La Iglesia and La Colonia (Figure 1). La Defensa is the area closest to the forest, located 200 m from it. La Iglesia is located in the center of the village, about 2.5 km away from the forest, and La Colonia, situated on the outskirts of the village, is the farthest from the forest (4.4 km) and closer to the city of Santa Ana Chiautempan (2.2 km).

Tlalcuapan has 3,613 people spread over 837 households, 45% of the population is between 20 and 59 years of age, 10% is older than 60, and 13% of the population still speaks the Nahua (native language) 24.

Fieldwork and Interviews

Before this research started, we requested permission from the local authorities of the community to conduct it, notifying them about the project and requesting the consent of each interviewee.

Field trips were undertaken in the 2017 rainy season, accompanied by local people who knew the local mushrooms (hongueros), to learn the uses and names of the mushrooms, their seasons and where they grew. The mushrooms collected were identified taxonomically and deposited in the TLXM herbarium.

A semi-structured interview 25 was designed that included three aspects: (a) the respondent's personal information, including name, gender, origin, age and occupation; (b) edible mushroom-free listing, where each person was asked to name 20 mushroom he or she knew; (c) questions about each mushroom’s bio-cultural importance (use, who uses it, how many times per week it is used, since when it has been used, way to get the mushrooms, harvesting activity, current or obsolete activity, its role in the forest and local management of mushroom collection and declared significance.

The interviews were divided into three groups of 45 individuals each. Each group corresponded to the areas in which the community is divided. The groups were composed of people of different ages, ranging from 8 to 90 years, which were classified into three age ranges: youth (8-20 years old), adults (21-60 years old) and older adults (persons over 60 years); each age range consisted of 15 individuals. In total, 135 people were interviewed, representing 15% of the main households of Tlalcuapan.

Statistical Analysis

The data obtained was analyzed with descriptive statistics to determine the importance that mushrooms have in the community. Two analyses of Multidimensional Non-Metric Scaling (NMDS) were applied to calculate two indices: a) Ethnotaxa Index (EI), to group persons according to the mushrooms mentioned in the free listing task, B) Bio-Cultural Significance Index (BCSI), to evaluate the Cultural significance of mushrooms, between the interviewed persons.

A qualitative matrix (46 X 135 OTUs or persons) was constructed with mushroom local names, age and home location of each person (from the three studied areas) to calculate de EI. In the case of BCSI, another qualitative matrix (29 X 135 OTUs or persons) was constructed, using the third section of the interview data, age and home location of each person (from the three studied areas). Responses to each question were coded as binary variables 26. A similitude analysis was carried out using the Euclidian distance index, followed by a NMDS Ordination analysis. So that each individual's scores represent each index. All eigenvalues, their percentages and their cumulative percentages were obtained. The age and origin of the interviewed persons were used to explain the scores obtained by them in each index (EI, BCSI). This relationship was assessed using models of multiple linear regression 27, with the score of each person in the index as the dependent variable, and the age and origin as explanatory variables. Finally, a third linear regression was developed to evaluate the relationship between EI and BCSI. All analyses were carried out in the program R 28.

Results

Relevance of Edible Wild Mushrooms in SPT

The people of Tlalcuapan use 70 traditional names for the edible wild mushrooms found in the area, of which 39 are in Spanish and 31 in Nahua. These names correspond to 46 ethnotaxa and 50 species (Table 1). Most of the people interviewed (80%) identified more than two edible mushrooms. The mushrooms most often mentioned were Russuladelica complex, Amanita basii and Boletus aff. edulis.

Table 1. Wild mushroom name, tradicional name and place of growth known in SPT. Organization based to the frecuency mentioned.
Scientific Name Common Spanish name Common Nahualt name Mention frequency Vegetation and ground use
P Q G A B C
Russulabrevipes PeckRussulacomplex. Delica Fr. Tecajete TecaxTecax 102102 XX   X      
Amanita basiiGuzmán & Ram.-Guill Amarillo, Flor(Yellow, flower) Ayoxochitl 97 X          
Boletus complex. Edulis Bull.   Pante 92 X   X      
Ramaria spp. Escobeta (Broom) Xelwas 73 X          
Hebeloma aff. mesophaeum (Pers.) Quél. Xolete de chambusquina (Xolete from burned areas) Xoletl 70         X  
Cantharellusaff.cibariusFr.   Tecosa 65   X X      
LaccariatrichodermophoraG.M. Muell.   Xoxocoyuli 64 X          
Suilluspseudobrevipes A.H. Sm. & Thiers Pancita(Little belly) Poposo 61 X X        
Turbinellusfloccosus(Schwein.) Singer Corneta(Trumpet) Tlapitzal 52       X    
AgaricuscampestrisL. Llanero, Hongo de pasto (Plain or grass mushroom) Ayutzi 48           X
Lyophyllumaff. decastes(Fr.) Singer.   Tzenzo 48   X        
Lactariusindigo (Schwein.)Fr. Tecax azul(Blue tray) Tlapaltecax 47 X X        
Pleorotusopuntiae(Durieu & Léville) Sacc. Hongo de maguey (Maguey mushroom) Mesonanatl 38           X
Chroogomphusjamaicensis(Murrill) O.K. Mill. Borracho(Drunk) Tlapaltecosa 24 X X        
Hygrophoruschrysodon (Batsch) Fr. Señorita, Palomita(Miss, Little dove) Totoltenanacatl 24 X          
LycoperdonperlatumPers. Huevito blanco, Tzefamil chico (White little egg, Little tzefamil) Popote 24 X   X      
CalvatiacyathiformisFr.   Tzefamil 22           X
559435111569500Rhizopogon aff. michoacanicus Trappe & Guzmán Huevito (Little egg) Xitetl 21 X       X  
Amanita sp.   Cuehcuex 20           X
Hypomyceslactifluorum (Schwein.) Tul. & C. Tul. Chilnanzi naranja (Orange chilnantzi) Chilnanzi, Chilnanacatl 20 X          
Hypomycesmacrosporus Seaver Chilnanzi café (Brown chilnanzi) Chilnanzi, Chilnanacatl 19 X          
Reticularialycoperdon Bull. (Myxomycete) Caca de luna(Moon shit) Cuahtechol 13 X          
Lactariusdeliciosus (L.) Gray Enchiladito(With chile) Tlapaltecax, Ocotecax 12 X          
Amanita amerifulva Tulloss Venadito, casco de soldado (Little deer, soldier’s helmet)   11 X          
Auriculariaauricula-judae Bull. Oreja ratón de tronco (Mouse ear’s trunk) Quimixnacas 8       X    
Lyophyllum sp.   Oco-Tzenzo 7 X          
Morchellasnyderi M. Kuo & Methven Chipotle   7       X    
AgaricusbisporusJ.E. Lange Champiñon   6            
Ustilagomaydis (DC.) Corda   Cuitlacoche 5           X
Infundibulicybegibba Pers. Cueros, sombrillitas (Leather, Little umbrella) Nacas cuero 4 X X   X    
Macrolepiotaaff. procera (Scop.) Singer   Tulnanacatl 4 X         X
Amanita complex.rubescens Pers Mantequilla(Butter)   3 X          
Armillariacomplex.mellea   Xopitzal 3   X        
Infundibulicybesp.   Totomoxnanacatl 3   X        
Ramaria aff. suecica (Fr.) Donk Escobeta café, Escobilla de ocote(Brown broom) Xelhuas 3 X          
Ramaria cf. cystidiophora (Kauffman) Corner, Escobeta café(Brown broom) Xelhuas 3   X        
Tricholomaflavovirens (Pers.) S. Lundell Railita, Kailita   3 X          
Amanita elongataPeck Yema(Yolk)   2 X   X      
Gymnopuscomplex. dryophillus (Bull.) Murrill Xolete pata amarilla (Yellow foot xolete) Xoletl 2 X          
Helvella crispa Bull. Oreja de ratón, Soldadito (Mouse ear’s, soldier) Xocuepich 2 X X        
Laccaria sp.   Xoxocoyuli cihuatl 2 X X        
Lactariussalmonicolor R. Heim & Leclair Enchilado de oyamel (Mushroom with chile from fir) Ayometecax 2       X    
Ramariaaff.rasilisporaMarr & D.E. Stuntz Escobeta amarilla(Yellow broom) Xelhuas 2   X        
Ramariarubricarnata Marr & D.E. Stuntz Escobeta amarilla(Yellow broom) Xelhuas 2   X        
Ramariarubripermanens Marr & D.E. Stuntz Escobeta rosa(Pink broom) Xelhuas 2   X        
Russulacomplex.xerampelina (Schaeff.) Fr. Pastelito(Little cake)   2 X          
Agaricus sp. Llanero de monte(Mountain Llanero)   1 X          
ClavariadelphustruncatusDonk. Acocote   1   X        
Pholiota lenta (Pers.) Singer Xolete de ocote(Pine’s xolete) Ocoxoletl 1 X          
Total mushroom by vegetation and ground use 30 15 5 5 2 6

* P = pine forest,
O = oak forest,
G = grassland,
A = fir forest,
B = burned areas,
C = cropland.

The data shows that people living in the area of La Defensa (the closest to the forest) were able to name more mushrooms, compared to the people in the other areas. The results also show that, regardless of the place of origin, older people can identify more types of mushrooms than the younger ones can (Figure 2).

Figure 2.Number of edible mushrooms mentioned by each age range in the three areas of the community.
 Number of edible mushrooms mentioned by each age range in the three areas of the                       community.

The young people named, in total, 30 mushrooms. The young person who recognized more species, mentioned a maximum of 21. In this age group, 71% identified 6 mushroom or fewer. Among the adults, in total, 39 different mushrooms were identified, 56% identifying 8 or fewer mushrooms. It is notable, however, that at least 20% of the adults identified 31 or more mushrooms. Older adults named a total of 40 mushrooms, and 76% knew 16 mushrooms or more (Figure 3).

Figure 3.Percentage of people who identify the mushrooms referred to in each of the age categories.
 Percentage of people who identify the mushrooms referred to in each of the age categories.

People in the community identify six areas and/or types of vegetation where the different edible mushrooms are collected: (1) pine (Pinus) forest, (2) oak (Quercus) forest, (3) oyamel (Abies) forest), (4) Zacatón (grassland of Stipa ichu), (5) agricultural land and (6) burn sites. The Pinus forest has the greatest abundance of mushrooms (30 spp.), followed by the Quercus forest (15 spp.) and the agricultural land (Table 1).

The interviews show that people get the most mushrooms by direct harvesting. Only 38% of the population purchases them and 5% does not eat them. People who harvest mushroom live mostly in La Defensa and in the La Iglesia region (Table 2).

Table 2. Perspective about mushroom harvesting and consumption, by different age groups in the three main areas of San Pedro Tlalcuapan.
Area Age Group Buy* Harvest Weekly consume Participate during harvesting Age of activity
Past Past/present
La Defensa Youth 2 14 2 13 2 13
Adults 4 14 2 14 1 14
Old adults 4 15 3 15 0 15
La Iglesia Youth 5 11 1 8 5 10
Adults 12 12 1 13 2 13
Old adults 11 12 1 14 2 13
La Colonia Youth 8 8 0 7 5 10
Adults 11 6 1 12 5 10
Old adults 11 9 1 15 4 11

*Buy: this column indicates the number of people interviewed who buy the mushrooms. Harvest: Indicates how many people collect them. Weekly consume: Is the average of how many times per week eat mushrooms. Participate during harvesting: Indicates the number of interviewees who participate in the harvest. Age of activity: Number of interviewees who consider the collection and consumption of fungi as obsolete or as something in force.

During mushroom season, eating mushrooms is very common in these communities, with locals eating them about twice a week, although it does vary depending on the area where people live. In La Defensa, young people and adults consume mushrooms, on average, two times a week, but older adults eat them three times a week. In La Iglesia, all three age ranges ate mushrooms at least once a week and, in the case of the area farthest from the forest (La Colonia), young people no longer eat them, whereas adults and older people eat mushrooms on average once a week (Table 2).

During fieldwork in the forest, it was noted that collection is a family activity, involving at least two generations, which includes children, young people, adults and the elderly, and the transmission of knowledge about mushrooms is mostly consecutive. In other words it is passed along from the grandparents to their children and grandchildren, and so on, from generation to generation.

(Table 2) shows that the people of La Defensa participate in mushrooms gathering regardless of age (young people, adults and the elderly), but the participation of children decreases at more distant forest sites. Most people (75% of respondents) consider collecting mushrooms as being a part of current ancestral knowledge and when you get farther away from the forest (La Iglesia and La Colonia), it is considered to be antiquity (Table 2).

During fieldwork in the forest and in the interviews, the fruiting season of each ethnotaxa was recorded. The mushroom season comprises six months, running from May to October, with July and August being the time of the greatest abundance of ethnotaxa (Table 3).

Table 3. Wild mushrooms growing during the season (May-October 2017), in SPT, Tlaxcala.
Mushroom species May June July August September October
Agaraicus campestris X          
Reticularia lycoperdon X          
Pleurotus opuntiae X X        
Calvatia cyathiformis X X        
Rhizopogoncomplex aff. michoacanicus X X        
Hebeloma aff. mesophaeum X X        
Agaricus bisporus X X X X X X
Lyophyllum aff. decastes   X X      
Amanita sp.   X X      
Lyophyllum sp.   X X      
Macrolepiota aff. procera   X X      
Lactarius aff. salmonicolor   X X X    
Amanita basii   X X X    
Pholiota lenta   X X X    
Boletus aff. edulis   X X X X  
Suillus pseudobrevipes   X X X X  
Lactarius indigo   X X X X  
Lactarius deliciosus   X X X X  
Ramaria sp.   X X X X  
Laccaria trichodermophora   X X X X  
Russulaxerampelina complex.   X X X X  
Hypomyces lactifluorum   X X X X X
Hypomyces macrosporus   X X X X X
Infundibulicybe gibba   X X X X X
Lycoperdon perlatum   X X X X X
Russula brevipes   X X X X X
Russuladelica complex.   X X X X X
Chroogomphus jamaicensis   X X X X X
Helvella crispa   X X X X X
Amanita amerifulva   X X X X X
Cantharellus aff. cibarius     X X X X
Turbinellus floccosus     X X X X
Hygrophorus chrysodon     X X X X
Laccaria sp.     X X X X
Tricholoma flavovirens     X X X X
Amanita elongata     X X X X
Ustilago maydis     X X X  
Hebeloma aff. mesophaeum     X X X  
Amanita rubescens complex     X X X  
Armillariamellea complex     X X    
Agaricus sp     X X    
Infundibulicybe sp.       X X  
Clavariadelphus truncatus       X X  
Auricularia auricula-judae         X X
Morchella snyderi           X

As for the role that mushrooms play in the forest, the people interviewed considered their greatest contribution to be a fertilizer for trees, as well as producing seeds to make more mushrooms in the following seasons. They are also food for forest animals and, to a lesser extent, act as disintegrator. Ninety-one percent of those interviewed recognize that there is a close relationship between mushrooms and trees.

Furthermore, three major “controlling” strategies were mentioned as ways to preserve mushrooms. The first is that, when harvesting, one should leave a part of the mushroom in the place where it is found; another is not to harvest all of the mushrooms growing in an area; and another is that to collect them, then clean and shake them where they are picked to promote their dispersion. Only 42% of the respondents -- mostly people living in La Defensa – actually do this (Table 4).

Table 4. Wild mushroom relevance in the forest and traditional use in different areas of San Pedro Tlalcuapan and age groups.
Areas Age group Wild mushroom in the forest Relationship mushroom/plant Traditional use
Fertilizer Animal food Seed Desintegrator Leave part of the mushroom Leaves some mushrooms Cleans out all the area
La Defensa Youth 11 1 4 0 14 6 0 2
Adults 8 3 8 0 13 10 1 3
Old adults 8 1 7 0 15 11 0 2
La Iglesia Youth 5 4 2 0 13 5 0 0
Adults 6 2 9 1 14 5 2 0
Old adults 8 2 7 0 14 5 0 1
La Colonia Youth 6 7 2 0 11 3 0 0
Adults 5 3 5 0 14 2 1 0
Old adults 8 1 7 0 14 3 0 0

Finally, when respondents considered the detailed relevance of wild mushrooms, they thought that they are important because they are food and they have ecological and economic value (Figure 4).

Figure 4.Importance of wild mushrooms for inhabitants of the community of Tlalcuapan.
 Importance of wild mushrooms for inhabitants of the community of Tlalcuapan.

Ethnotaxa and Bio-Cultural Significance Index

The result of the NMDS performed with a free listing of the mushrooms named by the respondents is acceptable (stress 0.13), the resulting ordination analysis shows a greater dispersion of the observations along axis 1, than the axis 2 (with values of between -1.6 to 3.35) (Figure 5).

Figure 5.Non-metric multidimensional scaling (NMDS) that shows the distribution in the axis of the inhabitants of the different areas of SPT, according to their age category and its relationship with the regional diversity of mushroom. Triangles represent people from La Defensa, the diamonds to La Iglesia and squares to people of La Colonia. White makes reference to young people, gray to adults and black to older adults. The solid round dots allow to locate the ethnotaxa.
 Non-metric multidimensional scaling (NMDS) that shows the distribution in the axis of the inhabitants of the different areas of SPT, according to their age category and its relationship with the regional diversity of mushroom. Triangles represent people from La Defensa, the           diamonds to La Iglesia and squares to people of La Colonia. White makes reference to young people, gray to adults and black to older adults. The solid round dots allow to locate the                   ethnotaxa.

Ten more wild mushroom mentioned are ordered at the positive end of the axis 1, while rare mushrooms are found in the negative end. This order suggests a positive relationship between this axis and the diversity of mushrooms that people are familiar with (ethnotaxa index). Thus, high values on the one axis indicate knowledge of the greater diversity of wild mushrooms (Figure 5).

As for the age of the interviewees, young people were found in the negative part of Axis 1, implying a lesser knowledge of ethnotaxa within this demographic age range. Older adults, regardless of where they live, are notably separated from young people, having positive rates of ethnotaxa, i.e., having a greater knowledge of mushrooms.

Where interviewees live is also a variable that has an impact on the grouping, with most of the La Defensa interviewees showing positive index values (between 0.003 and 3.34), corresponding to the interviewees who mentioned the majority of mushrooms. The people of La Colonia (values between - 1.6 and 0) know less about mushrooms including in their responses a cultivated white mushroom known as “el Champiñon”. Because of this, they have a lower rate of ethnotaxa. Those who live in the area of La Iglesia have values ranging from - 1.6 and 3.09 as they share knowledge in both areas.

The NMDS applied to the data from the interviews on bio-cultural relevance is acceptable (stress 0.25), showing a greater variability associated with Axis 1, with values between - 1.5 and 3 (Figure 6). Given the nature of the data, this analysis incorporates the importance and use of mushrooms.

Figure 6.Non-metric multidimensional scaling (NMDS) showing the distribution on the axis of the inhabitants of the different areas of SPT, according to their age range and its relationship with mushrooms bio-cultural knowledge. Triangles represent people from La Defensa, the diamonds to La Iglesia and the squares to the people of La Colonia. White makes reference to young people, gray color to adults and black to older adults. The solid round dots allow to locate the ethnotaxa.
 Non-metric multidimensional scaling (NMDS) showing the distribution on the axis of the inhabitants of the different areas of SPT, according to their age range and its relationship with mushrooms bio-cultural knowledge. Triangles represent people from La Defensa, the diamonds to La Iglesia and the squares to the people of La Colonia. White makes reference to young                  people, gray color to adults and black to older adults. The solid round dots allow to locate the ethnotaxa.

The negative values of Axis 1 are a result of most of the variables related to ancestral knowledge and handling of the mushrooms, such as harvesting, consumption, forest functions and control strategies.

Three variables are located on the positive side of the Axis 1: purchase, disintegrator and animal food. For example, the people who buy mushrooms are put into a group that does not know where to find them or how to harvest them. As well, some consider mushrooms to be important only as food for animals and as a disintegrator in the forest and not as a useful resource for human beings.

It is observed that the variables that represent an extensive knowledge of mushrooms are grouped toward the negative side of the axis. Thus, a lower score on the axis of one of the variables of perception-based NMDS represents a greater bio-cultural relevance.

The majority of interviewees living in La Defensa are grouped in the negative part of Axis 1, which corresponds to a greater knowledge of mushrooms. The people of La Colonia, however, are grouped on the positive side, while the people of La Iglesia are mostly located in the middle part of the axis to correspond with their territorial location. With respect to the age groupings, there is no clear distribution the only age group that clusters together is the one comprising older people, which is located toward the negative side of the axis.

Change in Local Knowledge According to age and the Territory

The models of multiple linear regression were adjusted to explain ethnotaxa rates and bio-cultural relevance according to repondents’ age and their area of origin. Both were significant (see Table 5 and Table 6). According to the coefficients estimated for the ethnotaxa index, the index changed depending on the age of the respondent: the older he or she gets, the higher the rate of ethnotaxa index, being 0.03 the reason for the change. The ethnotaxa index also varies according to the area where a person lives: those living nearest to the forest will have a higher rate (see Table 5). Although the value of the contribution of each of the variables was significant (p = 0.001), the model presents a relatively low r2 (r2 = 0.52), indicating that the regression explains the pattern of the data, but does not have predictive power.

Table 5. Multiple regression on the rate of ethnotaxa according to the place where respondents live and their according to their age.
  Estimated value T Value P Value
INTERCEPT -1.94 -9.93 < 0.001
LA DEFENSA 1.57 8.29 < 0.001
LA IGLESIA 0.64 3.39 < 0.001
AGE 0.03 8.72 < 0.001
r2= 0.52

The index of bio-cultural relevance is also explained by age and where respondents live. The model indicates that this index changes with the age of the individual; the reason for change is - 0.005, i.e., on average an individual who is one year older than another will have a lower rate (lower than 0.005). Thus, older people tend to have the most negative relevance bio-cultural indices, which is confirmed with a reliability of 95%. The index of cultural relevance is also significantly related (p = 0.01) to the area where people live: according to the estimated coefficients, people living in La Colonia obtained a score of 0.66, for people that live in La Iglesia or in La Defensa decreased in 0.39 and 0.96 points, respectively (coefficients negative, Table 6). Therefore, the respondents who live nearest the forest will have a lower rate, i.e., a greater knowledge about mushrooms (Table 6).

Table 6. Multiple regression on relevance of bio-cultural knowledge depending on where people live and their age index.
  ESTIMATED VALUE T VALUE MEANING
INTERCEPT 0.66 4.85 < 0.001
LA DEFENSA -0.96 -7.28 < 0.001
LA IGLESIA -0.39 -2.93 0.01
AGE -0.005 -2.24 0.05
r2= 0.31

Association Between the Indices

Regression analysis from the axis was significant (Figure 7). According to the estimated coefficient, it suggests that for every unit of decline in the ethnotaxa index, there is a 1.05 increase in the value of the index of bio-cultural relevance (p < 0.001). While this regression significantly explains the contribution of relevant bio-cultural index values, the r2 is relatively low (r2 = 0.37), indicating that they do not have any predictive value. As shown in Figure 7, there is abundant variability in the data. Both indices explain the differences that exist in the ethnomycological knowledge between the age groups and where people live, but one finding cannot be predicted based on the other.

Figure 7.Regression on the index of ethnotaxa according to biocultural relevance, p index < 0.001, r2 = 0.37.
 Regression on the index of ethnotaxa according to biocultural relevance, p index < 0.001,  r2 = 0.37.

Discussion

The data obtained show that, in SPT, mushrooms are an important natural resource for the community, since the majority of the population identify and use them. The richness in the variety of names that are given to the wild mushrooms by specific ethnic groups is one of the indicators of the importance of these species, and the many traditional mushroom names correspond to a greater knowledge about their use and biology 29. SPT has more useful mushroom species (50) than other communities situated on La Malinche 18, 19, 30. Although they have reported about 15 to 30 species of mushrooms, the number of species in the community studied is similar to that reported for the Nahua community of San Isidro Buensuceso, where 48 species have been recorded 30.

Two of the species most frequently mentioned by respondents to this study (Amanita basii and Boletus edulis complex.) largely agree with those recognized as the most important in other communities of La Malinche, Tlaxcala and other regions of Mexico 19, 31, 32, 33. This confirms the importance of these mushroom species in Mexico. The preference of Russuladelica complex, is a special trait that distinguish to this community.

The majority of the people who consume edible wild mushroom mentioned in this study had been eating them for at least six months per year. Many of the respondents (mainly those who live in the two areas closest to the forest) obtain the mushrooms by harvesting them directly. This activity, as well as how the mushrooms are used, is still passed along from generation to generation. As is suggested by Boesch and Tomasello 34, this type of cultural transmission occurs between individuals of different generations, but within the same family (vertical transmission), for example, from parents to children. Furthermore, it is observed that this transmission is oral and hands-on, as mentioned by Luna-Morales 35, who showed that traditional knowledge has been generated, selected and accumulated collectively over thousands of years, and lives on in the memory and practices of the people and is mainly passed on through oral and hands-on communications.

The residents of SPT also recognize the roles that wild mushrooms play in the forest, such as providing fertilizer, seed for new growth and food for wild animals, so, the mushrooms are considered an important part of the natural environment in which they grow. This is confirmed because many of the interviewees noted that mushrooms are associated with trees and, over the years, they have used different methods to maintain their care.

Some studies in which traditional mycological knowledge is described mention that this knowledge varies depending on age 36, 37, 38, 23 and gender 19, 20, 21, however, in most of these studies they compare the number of fungi and uses that people enunciate, without considering other variables or statistical analyzes that allow identifying the differences between age ranges. An exception is that of Pacheco-Cobos et al. 20 who GPS-tracked the foraging pathways of 21 pairs of men and women from an indigenous Mexican community searching for mushrooms in a natural environment; measures of costs, benefits and general search efficiency were analyzed and related to differences between the two sexes in foraging patterns.

In this case, the two indices proposed in this study show significant differences in the knowledge about mushrooms depending on the age of, or the area inhabited by, the people of SPT. The EI is useful because it takes into account the mushrooms that everyone knows and not just their number allowing distinguishing those exclusive of certain age ranges. On the other hand, the BCSI considers different variables about uses, collection and mushroom ecology, that indicate the importance of the mushrooms as a resource, what facilitated knowing the knowledge distribution.

The age was an important factor in the level of knowledge, as it was observed in other studies 12, 15, 39. Regression analysis demonstrated that older people have a higher ethnotaxa index, i.e., they can identify the highest number of wild mushrooms in the area. But also a lower BCSI, which indicates that the elderly have a greater ecological knowledge, about collection and more uses than young people.

This situation could be explained by, as suggested by Garro 40, that age is associated with the natural process of the acquisition of knowledge, i.e., the greater the awareness in older adults is attributed to the greater opportunities they have had to learn, acquiring more experience and contact with the natural resources around them 15. Also several studies have shown that the peak of acquisition of teoric knowledge, i.e. when this knowledge is similar to an adult, occurs at 10-15 years old 41, 42, 43, with an important increasing between 9-12 years old 44, 45, 46. There are another factors affecting the acquisition process.

The fact the children and young people know less and participating less in these collection activities, indicate a loss in the transmission of mushroom knowledge, specially in La Colonia area. Eyssartier et al. 47, found that the transmission of traditional knowledge begins in early childhood, when children accompany their parents in cultivating the land. It appears, however, that, today, there are fewer opportunities for children to spend time with their parents, grandparents and others who know the practices and belives about conservation 48. It has documented that formal education marginalizes local knowledge 14, 49, causing changes and promoting an urban life style 50, 12.

The study findings also suggest that lower level of knowledge among young people may be result of lack of interest in traditional knowledge, as young people – influenced by modernization -- may well consider this type of knowledge to be obsolete. It has, indeed, been demonstrated that older people are less affected by external influences than the young 13. Young people tend to abandon their homes and ancestral customs as they focus their interests ton meeting the demands of Western culture 50.

The EI shows that people of each range age mention exclusive mushrooms, for example, Champiñon (Agaricusbisporus) is the most popular between young people. Yellow foot xolete (Gymnopusdryophillus) and Little cake (Russulaxerampelina complex) were named only by adults, whereas the Ayometecax (Lactariussalmonicolor), Tzunanacatl (Macrolepiota aff. procera) and Xocuepix (Helvellacrispa) were mentioned by older adults.

Significant differences were observed between the three areas of the community. The BCSI, permitted the observation that most of the people living the furthest away from the forest have less mushroom knowledge, since them do not collect, eat fewer of them and buy the mushroom what they do eat. They do not know where the mushrooms grow or how to take care of them. Because these people live in an urban area, they seem to think it easier to go to the grocery store to buy the farmed white mushrooms (Champiñon) than to go to the forest to look for the wild ones. Bonilla-Moheno and García-Frapolli 51 as well as, Reyes-García et al. 52 mention that the integration to the Market is an important factor that modifying traditional knowledge because people who can pay these kind of market products, substitute the forests products, making traditional knowledge irrelevant. Most of the people living in La Defensa close to the forest eat mushrooms which they themselves have picked, which increases local knowledge about mushrooms, where they grow, what season they appear and their uses. In addition, the majority says that it is a complementary activity to their daily work in agriculture, involving the whole family. Other authors have noted this also 14, 15 people who work with the environment are able to maintain a greater knowledge of mushrooms than those who do not.

For La Defensa residents, it is more economical and rewarding to get food from the mountain instead of going to the supermarket in the city, since that would incur major expenses. Many people said "we do not know the quality of what we buy and, in the forest, everything is clean and healthier.” This is why they continue eating wild mushrooms.

The people of La Iglesia share both perceptions. Although the regression analysis indicates that each area has a different view of wild mushrooms, it confirms that the distance of housing from the forest has a significant impact on traditional knowledge. This indicates that there is a loss of mycological knowledge among people who live far from the forest and closer to the city.

While the bio-cultural relevance index includes more variables analyzed and, therefore, raises several questions, both indices were useful for achieving the objectives of this study, allowing the comparison of mushroom knowledge among local sites in the community and age groups. In addition, this study makes a methodological contribution since it can be replicated in any other community or group of communities. It must be taken into account, however, that neither of the indices can predict the outcome of the other. So future studies need to establish clear objectives and decide which index to use or perhaps develop one that it encompasses both objectives, since these data are independent.

Conclusions

SPT is a community in which currently the wild mushroom are a resource of great importance; however, previous analyses, and as authorities and residents of SPT have stated, there is a risk that traditional wild mushroom knowledge will vanish, as it seems to be restricted to only a part of the community’s territory and to particular age groups, indicating that there is a fragmentation in the transmission of knowledge from generation to generation. This study will allow researchers to focus on how to better disseminate this knowledge to the vulnerable age groups and suggest strategies of strengthening the transmission of this knowledge to other age groups and/or territories.

On the other hand, the proposed indices facilitate the evaluation of the variables associated with the distribution of traditional knowledge and allow comparisons among different areas of study. These are also flexible, since the bio-cultural variables can be included or eliminated and adjusted to different cultures or natural resources.

Authors' Contributions

EB-C designed the study, carried out the field work, the analysis and interpretation of data and wrote the manuscript. AC-M made substantial contributions to the analysis and interpretation of data. RIT-V, AM and JC participated in the design of the study, and the revision of the document. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a doctorate grant assigned to Eribel Bello by CONACyT (number of award 290010) and the "Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México", was also supported by the project PAPIIT IN301118. We are very grateful to the people of SPT for their hospitality, interest and enthusiasm in participating in this research, especially to the families of Mrs. Mary, Mrs. Maura, Mrs. Tomasita, Mrs. Felix, Doña Teodora, Mario Orlando and Angel Eduardo who shared their knowledge to us and supported in the forest fieldwork. We are equally grateful to Eleazar Bello Cervantes, who shared their knowledge about the statistical analysis of data. Thanks to Ing. Elizabeth Hernández and Gundi Jeffrey, by critically reviewing the English version of this manuscript.

References

  1. 1.Berkes F, Colding J, Folke C. (2000) Rediscovery of traditional ecological knowledge as adaptive management. , Ecol Appl 10(5), 1251-1262.
  1. 2.Boege E. (2008) El patrimonio biocultural de los pueblos indígenas de México. Hacia la conservación in situ de la biodiversidad y agrodiversidad en los territorios indígenas. 1era ed. México: Instituto Nacional de Antropología e Historia, Comisión Nacional para el Desarrollo de los Pueblos Indígenas .
  1. 3.Jarblad A. (2003) The global politic economy of transnational corporation: a theory of asymetrhic interdependence. , Lulea of University Technology, C Ektended Essay 47.
  1. 4.V M Toledo, Barrera-Bassols N. (2008) La memoria biocultural. La importancia ecológica de las sabidurías tradicionales. 1era ed. España: Icaria editorial.
  1. 5.González-Espinosa M, Ramírez-Marcial N, Galindo-Jaimes L, Camacho-Cruz A, Golicher D. (2009) Tendencias y proyecciones del uso del suelo y la diversidad florística en Los Altos de Chiapas, México. Investigación Ambiental. Ciencia y Política Pública. 1(1), 40-53.
  1. 6.Maffi L. (2005) Linguistic, Cultural, and Biological Diversity. , Annual Rev Anthropol 34, 599-618.
  1. 7.Zent S, Maffi L. (2010) Methodology for Developing a Vitality Index of Traditional Environmental Knowledge (VITEK) for the Project “Global Indicators. of the Status and Trends of Linguistic Diversity and Traditional Knowledge.” Final Report on Indicator No. 2. British , Columbia, Canada: Terralingua .
  1. 8.Reyes-García V, Guèze M, Luz A C, Paneque-Gálvez J, M J. (2013) Evidence of traditional knowledge loss among a contemporary indigenous society. , Evol Hum Behav 34(4), 249-257.
  1. 9.Godoy R, Reyes-García V, Byron E, W R Leonard, Vadez V. (2005) The effect of market economies on the well-being of indigenous peoples and on their use of renewable natural resources.AnnRevAnthropol. 34, 121-138.
  1. 10.Gómez‐Baggethun E, Mingorría S, Reyes‐Garcia V, Calvet L, Montes C. (2010) Traditional ecological knowledge trends in the transition to a market economy: empirical study in the Doñana natural areas. , Conserv Biol 24(3), 721-729.
  1. 11.Aswani S, Lemahieu A, W H Sauer. (2018) Global trends of local ecological knowledge and future implications. , PloS One 13(4).
  1. 12.Saynes-Vásquez A, Caballero J, Meave J A, Chiang F. (2013) Cultural change and loss of ethnoecological knowledge among the Isthmus Zapotecs of Mexico. , J Ethnobiol Ethnomed 2-10.
  1. 13.Quinlan M B, Quinlan R J. (2007) Modernization and medicinal plant knowledge in a caribbean horticultural village. , Med Anthropol 21, 169-192.
  1. 14.Zent S. (2001) Acculturation and ethnobotanical knowledge loss among the Piaroa of Venezuela. Demonstration of a quantitative method for the empirical study of traditional environmental knowledge change. In: Maffi L (ed) On biocultural diversity. Linking language knowledge, and the environment. Smithsonian Institution Press. , Washington 190-211.
  1. 15.Silva F, M A Ramos, Hanazaki N, Albuquerque U P. (2011) Dynamics of traditional knowledge of medicinal plants in a rural community in the Brazilian semi-arid region. , Rev Bras Farmacogn 21(3), 382-391.
  1. 16.Dighton J. (2016) . Fungi in ecosystem processes.CRCPressUSA
  1. 17.Boa E. (2004) Wild edible fungi: a global overview of their use and importance to people. Food and Agriculture Org.
  1. 18.Montoya A, Estrada-Torres A, Caballero J. (2002) Comparative ethnomycological survey of three localities from La Malinche Volcano. , Mexico, J Ethnobiol 22, 103-131.
  1. 19.Montoya A, Torres-García E A, Kong A, Estrada-Torres A, Caballero J. (2012) Gender differences and regionalization of the cultural significance of wild mushrooms around La Malinche Volcano. , Tlaxcala, Mexico, Mycologia 104(4), 826-834.
  1. 20.Pacheco-Cobos L, Rosetti M F, Cuatianquiz C, Hudson R. (2010) Sex differences in mushroom gathering: men expend more energy to obtain equivalent benefits. , Evol Hum Behav 31(4), 289-297.
  1. 21.Burrola-Aguilar C, Montiel O, Garibay-Orijel R, Zizumbo-Villarreal L. (2012) Conocimiento tradicional y aprovechamiento de los hongos comestibles silvestres en la región de Amanalco, Estado de México. , Rev Mex Mic 35, 1-16.
  1. 22.F H Santiago, J P Moreno, B X Cázares, Suárez J J A, E O Trejo. (2016) Traditional knowledge and use of wild mushrooms by Mixtecs or Ñuu savi, the people of the rain, from Southeastern Mexico. , Journal of Ethnobiology and Ethnomedicine 12(1), 35.
  1. 23.Robles-García D, Suzán-Azpiri H, Montoya-Esquivel A, García-Jiménez J, E U. (2018) Ethnomycological knowledge in three communities in. , Amealco, Quéretaro, México, J Ethnobiol and Ethnomedicine 14(1), 7.
  1. 24.INEGI. (2010) Anuario geoestadístico de Tlaxcala.INEGIMéxico.
  1. 25.Alexiades M N. (1996) Collecting ethnobotanical data: An introduction to basic concepts and trchniques. In: Alexiades M (ed.) Selected guidelines for ethnobotanical reasearch: Afield Manual. , New York Botanical Garden, Nueva York
  1. 26.Legendre P, Legendre L. (2012) Numerical Ecology. Elsevier Scientific. , Oxford
  1. 27.Zar J H. (1999) Biostatistical Analysis, 4th ed. Upper Saddle River. , Prentice-Hall, NJ
  1. 28.R Core Team. (2016) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
  1. 29.Estrada-Torres A. (1989) La etnomicología: Avances, problemas y perspectivas. Examen predoctoral. México: Escuela Nacional de Ciencias Biológicas. , IPN
  1. 30.Montoya A, Hernández-Totomoch O, Estrada-Torres A, Kong A, Caballero J. (2003) Traditional knowledge about mushrooms in a Nahua community in the state of Tlaxcala. , Mexico, Mycologia 95, 793-806.
  1. 31.Montoya A, Kong A, Estrada-Torres A, Cifuentes J, Caballero J. (2004) Useful wild fungi of La Malinche National Park. , Mexico, Fung Divers 17, 115-143.
  1. 32.Garibay-Orijel R, Caballero J. (2007) Estrada-Torres A and. , Journal of Ethnobiology and Ethnomedicine 1-18.
  1. 33.L E Alonso-Aguilar, Montoya A, Kong A, Estrada-Torres A, Garibay-Orijel R. (2014) The cultural significance of wild mushrooms in San Mateo Huexoyucan. , Tlaxcala, Mexico, J Ethnobiol Ethnomed 10, 27.
  1. 34.Boesch C, Tomasello M. (1998) Chimpanzee and Human Cultures. , Curr Anthropol 39(5), 591-604.
  1. 35.Luna-Morales C. (2002) Ciencia, conocimiento tradicional y etnobotánica. , Etnobiología 2, 120-135.
  1. 36.Geng Y, Zhang Y, Ranjitkar S, Huai H, Wang Y. (2016) Traditional knowledge and its transmission of wild edibles used by the Naxi in Baidi Village, northwest Yunnan province. , J Ethnobiol Ethnomed 12(1), 10.
  1. 37.Fui F S, Saikim F H, Kulip J, JSS Seelan. (2018) Distribution and ethnomycological knowledge of wild edible mushrooms in Sabah (Northern Borneo). , Malaysia. J Trop Biol Conserv 15, 203-222.
  1. 38.H M Kamalebo, Malale H N S W, C M Ndabaga, Degreef J, A De Kesel. (2018) Uses and importance of wild fungi: traditional knowledge from the Tshopo province in the Democratic Republic of the Congo. , J Ethnobiol Ethnomed 14(1), 13.
  1. 39.Cortés-González J J. (2007) Variabilidad intracultural y pérdida del conocimiento sobre el entorno natural en una comunidad zapoteca del sur de México (Nizanda, Oaxaca). Tesis para obtener el grado de Maestro en Ciencias Biologicas, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México.
  1. 40.Garro L C. (1986) Intracultural variation in folk medical knowledge: A comparison between curers end noncurers. , Am Anthropol 88, 351-370.
  1. 41.Godoy R, Reyes-García V, Broesch J, I C Fitzpatrick, Giovannini P. (2009) Long-term (secular) change of ethnobotanical knowledge of useful plants: separating cohort and age effects. , J Anthropol Res 65(1), 51-67.
  1. 42.S M Carriére, Sabinot C, Pagezy H. (2017) Children´s Ecological knoeledge: grawings as a tool for ethnoecologists (Gabon. , Madagascar) AnthropoChildren
  1. 43.Tian X. (2017) Ethnobotanical knowledge acquisition during daily chores: the firewood collection of pastoral Maasai girls in. , Southern Kenya J Ethnobiol Ethnomed 13(1), 2.
  1. 44.Shenton J, Ross N, Kohut M, Waxman S. (2011) Maya Folk Botany and Knowledge Devolution: Modernization and Intra‐Community Variability in the Acquisition of Folkbotanical Knowledge. , Ethos 39(3), 349-367.
  1. 45.Demps K, Zorondo-Rodríguez F, García C, Reyes-García V. (2012) Social learning across the life cycle: cultural knowledge acquisition for honey collection among the Jenu Kuruba. , India. Evol Hum Behav 33(5), 460-470.
  1. 46.M B Quinlan, R J Quinlan, S K Council, J W Roulette. (2016) Children’s acquisition of ethnobotanical knowledge in a Caribbean horticultural village. , J Ethnobiol 36(2), 433-457.
  1. 47.Eyssartier C, Ladio A H, Lozada M. (2008) Cultural Transmission of Traditional Knowledge in two populations of North-western Patagonia. , Jour Ethnobiol Ethnomed 4(25), 8.
  1. 48.Berkes F, Turner N. (2006) Knowledge, Learning and the Resilience of Social-Ecological Systems. , Hum Ecol 34, 479-494.
  1. 49.Gómez-Baggethun E, Reyes-García V. (2013) Reinterpreting change in traditional ecological knowledge. , Hum Ecol 41(4), 643-647.
  1. 50.Ladio A H, Lozada M. (2003) Comparison of wild edible plant diversity and foraging strategies in two aboriginal communities of northwestern Patagonia. Biodivers Conserv12:. 937-951.
  1. 51.Bonilla-Moheno M, García-Frapolli E. (2012) Conservation in context: a comparison of conservation perspectives in a Mexican protected area. , Sustainability 4(9), 2317-2333.
  1. 52.Reyes-García V, Paneque-Gálvez J, A C Luz, Gueze M, M J. (2014) Cultural change and traditional ecological knowledge. An empirical analysis from the Tsimane’in the Bolivian Amazon. , Hum Organ 73(2), 162.

Cited by (6)

  1. 1.Ruan-Soto Felipe, Cifuentes Blanco Joaquín, Garibay Orijel Roberto, Caballero Nieto Javier, 2020, Comparación de la disponibilidad de hongos comestibles en tierras altas y bajas de Chiapas y sus implicaciones en las estrategias tradicionales de aprovechamiento, Acta Botanica Mexicana, (128), 10.21829/abm128.2021.1731
  1. 2.Torres-Gómez Mariano, Garibay-Orijel Roberto, Pérez-Salicrup Diego R., Casas Alejandro, Guevara Mario, 2023, Wild edible mushroom knowledge and use in five forest communities in central México, Canadian Journal of Forest Research, 53(1), 25, 10.1139/cjfr-2022-0043
  1. 3.Haro-Luna Mara Ximena, Blancas Vázquez José, Ruan-Soto Felipe, Guzmán-Dávalos Laura, 2022, Sociocultural drivers of mycological knowledge: insights from Wixarika and Mestizo groups in western Mexico, Journal of Ethnobiology and Ethnomedicine, 18(1), 10.1186/s13002-022-00564-2
  1. 4.Haro-Luna Mara Ximena, Ruan-Soto Felipe, Blancas José, Guzmán-Dávalos Laura, 2022, The cultural role played by the ethnomycological knowledge of wild mushrooms for the peoples of highlands and lowlands in Tlaltenango, Zacatecas, Mexico, Mycologia, 114(4), 645, 10.1080/00275514.2022.2068114
  1. 5.Eribel Bello-Cervantes, Irma Trejo, Fernanda Figueroa, Joaquín Cifuentes Blanco, 2023, Indigenous Traditional Knowledge on Wild Edible Mushrooms: Cultural Significance, Extraction Practices, and Factors Leading to Changes in Their Abundance in Central Mexico, Journal of Ethnobiology, 43(4), 351, 10.1177/02780771231211984
  1. 6.Thomas Paul W., Vazquez Luis-Bernardo, 2022, A novel approach to combine food production with carbon sequestration, biodiversity and conservation goals, Science of The Total Environment, 806(), 151301, 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2021.151301